[lit-ideas] The Descent of Man and self-sacrifice

  • From: "Lawrence Helm" <lawrencehelm@xxxxxxxxxxxxxx>
  • To: <lit-ideas@xxxxxxxxxxxxx>
  • Date: Fri, 23 May 2014 13:01:26 -0700

The argument Wade presented in the previous note was from Darwin’s Descent of 
Man and not from his Origin of Species.    

As to the argument that “we need to reject the idea that behaviour that removes 
an organism from the gene-pool will be 'selected for' because it benefits the 
remaining group - this simply does not work as a theory, because nothing can be 
'selected for' via its removal from the gene-pool.”  That can’t be true.  I’ve 
read several authors refer to organisms doing that very thing.  One early 
author, can’t recall his name, referred to a pair of adult baboon males 
guarding their tribes passage up through a narrow passage where they would be 
safe for the night.  The leopard came and they set upon it with precession.  
The leopard killed both of them, but before he did, one of them bit into the 
leopard’s jugular.  

I have read a number of similar accounts.  The theory advanced to account for 
this well-documented sort of thing is that while these baboons, for example 
(and who knows, maybe their genes were passed into the tribes gene pool before 
this event) were killed.  The tribe, including the near relatives of these 
baboons, lived on.  They didn’t protect their own genes, but they their genes, 
less any individual mutations would have lived on in the tribe.

 Wade goes on to write that Darwin’s ideas were “developed by Edward O. Wilson 
in his landmark 1975 book Sociobiology and extended from animals to people. 
‘The requirement for an evolutionary approach to ethics is self-evident,’ he 
wrote. Sociobiology, though intended by its author as merely a synthesis of new 
biological ideas, posed a political challenge to Marxists and much of the 
academic left. It showed how the human mind was not a blank slate, on which 
governments could write whatever ideological prescriptions they wished in order 
to shape Socialist Man, but was already shaped or predisposed by evolution to 
behave in certain ways. Wilson’s book was assailed by Marxist colleagues at 
Harvard, such as the geneticist Richard Lewontin. Students disrupted Wilson’s 
lectures and harassed even Hamilton and Trivers. Researchers dared not use the 
word sociobiology, even if they agreed with its ideas, lest they be caught up 
in the furor. Sociobiology, as applied to people, is now pursued mostly under 
the name of evolutionary psychology. Richard Alexander, after the storm over 
Sociobiology had settled, was one of the first biologists to resume the study 
of morality. Human ancestors lived in groups, he argued in a book published in 
1987, as a defense against other human groups, and warfare had been a major 
influence in human evolution. Usually predators find it most efficient to live 
in small groups (wolves, lions, killer whales) while it is prey animals that 
congregate in large herds for defense. But humans departed from this rule, 
probably because their most feared enemies were other human groups. Incessant 
warfare led to selection for greater social complexity and intelligence, and 
the larger societies required ever greater self-constraint to avoid infringing 
on other individuals’ interests, Alexander argued. “The function or raison d’ 
être of moral systems is evidently to provide the unity required to enable the 
group to compete successfully with other human groups. Only in humans is the 
major hostile force of life composed of other groups in the same species,” he 
wrote.

“The surprising idea that people might be inherently moral was difficult for 
biologists and others to accept because it conflicted with the usual assumption 
that human nature is selfish. Even harder to swallow, for those not steeped in 
the concepts of evolutionary biology, was the assertion that something as 
precious as morality could have blossomed from the murky soil of strife and 
warfare.”  [Wade, Nicholas (2009-10-27). The Faith Instinct: How Religion 
Evolved and Why It Endures (Kindle Locations 558-577). Penguin Group US. Kindle 
Edition.]

Wade’s purpose isn’t primarily to show that an individual’s moral instinct is 
sufficient to put the good of the pack above his own.  Too many examples of 
this exist for this to be questioned (at least by too many).  It is to 
demonstrate that we (and not just we but primates as well) inherited a moral 
instinct.  “In distress, they elicit sympathy with a range of very human 
expressions. “When upset, chimpanzees pout, whimper, yell, beg with 
outstretched hand, or impatiently shake both hands so that the other will hurry 
and provide the calming contact so urgently needed,” de Waal wrote. Chimps have 
been known to try to save others from drowning in the moats that sometimes 
surround zoo colonies. This is a huge risk for them because they cannot swim.”  
[Wade, Kindle Locations 593-597]

“So what then is morality? De Waal’s definition, from his perspective as a 
primatologist, is very different from that of rationalist philosophers. “We 
understand morality as a sense of right and wrong that is born out of 
group-wide systems of conflict management based on shared values,” he writes. 
“Moral systems thus provide a set of rules and incentives to resolve 
competition and conflicts within the group in the service of the ‘greater 
good,’ that is, benefits (to individuals) derived from resource distribution 
and collective action. Morality, by this definition, is closely related to 
social behavior.” By breaking out of the specialist frameworks in which 
philosophers and psychologists had long imprisoned the study of morality, De 
Waal established that morality is a biological behavior and that evolution is 
the only framework in which the origins of morality can be addressed. [Wade, 
Kindle Locations 607-614]

Lawrence

Other related posts: